AUCTORES
Research | DOI: https://doi.org/10.31579/2693-4779/057
*Corresponding Author: Antonis Theofilidis. School of Psychology- Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.
Citation: A Theofilidis, F Kargopoulos. (2021 Mind’s selective attention to previous experience. Clinical Research and Clinical Trials. 4(3); DOI: 10.31579/2693-4779/057
Copyright: © 2021 Antonis Theofilidis, This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Received: 05 August 2021 | Accepted: 21 August 2021 | Published: 01 September 2021
Keywords: visual- spatial skill; mental image; cognitive schemas; mentall partition
The basic arguments for a mental image model of thought are based on neuropsychological evidence.
France (2000) found that the same brain regions are activated during both mental representation and actual perception, while Bishiah (1993) found that brain traumas that affected perception, also affected the ability to create mental images. Pylyshyn(2003) on the other hand, argues that all mental images are guided by ‘’cognitive penetrability’’, thus on their very basis, are manipulated by certain propositional elements. Given this difficulty, Kargopoulos (2009) hinted towards further research, featuring shapes and solid objects, for which subjects have no priorextensive knowledge. This would force subjects to use non-semantic strategies of representation, meaning mental imagery. Hinton’s (1979) cube problem conforms to these requirements. Hinton’s problem aligns with the idea that spatial tasks (especially tasks with cubes that change layout) are guided by propositional cues (our knowledge about squares) and supports Pylyshyn’s position. Using one of the simplest objects, a cube, Hinton showed that as soon as this shape changes its mental arrangement in space, even suspicious -as to the nature of the experiment- participants will make mistakes that are not present when they manipulate a mental image of the cube sitting on its typicalarray.
Aim: Our goal was to investigate the relationship between spatio-visual skill and the ability for mental partitioning in healthy subjects.
Methodology: We used 2 groups (344 participants) a control and an experimental one. In the control group, we presented a Moebius’ strip, in the experimental group, we presented the same Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally represent it. All participants asked to mentally partition the strip.
Results: Of the 344 participants, only 31 managed to give the correct number of vertices in space. Though people had a hard time manipulating the cube’s mental image, their success rates were much higher for the Hinton 1 task in which propositional representation was more accessible. Only 9 of the 344 participants could find the correct answer for the Moebius strip task in which mental manipulation of the strip image was impossible.
Conclusions: We come to the conclusion that the relationship between ‘’seeing’’ and ‘’knowing’’ is more complex, not just on the level of the mental image level but also on the level of perception. Our findings bring back to the scientific background the idea that the mind’s selective attention to previous experience and cognitive schemas will decidedly affect human thought.
Mental Partitioning
Although a lot of studies in cognitive psychology have researched mental object manipulation or transformation, there have been only a few attempting to locate the limits of mental partitioning in objects. Taking into account the limited research on mental partitioning, no study has examined the underlying mechanisms of mental partitioning, and the limits of its revolving simple, understandable objects for which subjects didn’t have prior knowledge (ex. Moebius’ strip), as well as its relationship to performance in spatial tasks. Research has mainly focused on spatio-visual and constructive-visual skills drawing from different theoretical backgrounds and utilizing a wide array of experiment designs, samples, analysis and data processing methods. In any case, there remains a basic challenge for cognitive psychology: Are there any mental partitioning tasks that may hint towards the nature of mental representations (solving the Kosslyn – Pylyshyn dispute) and is the theory of cognitive penetrability sufficient to establish a new propositional theory of perception?
Moebius’ Strip
Moebius’ strip is the first surface with one layer to be discovered and studied. It is a surface with only one side and one boundary. We can create a Moebius’ strip by cutting a strip from a piece of paper, rotating it by 180o, and then joining its ends to form a loop. Using a Moebius’ strip in mental representation studies allows us toexamine people’s limits in space– perception tasks.
The current study used an easily depictable shape for which subjects didn’t have much prior knowledge. Moebius’strip conforms to the aforementioned characteristics, as it possesses a peculiar quality: partitioning the strip along its axon will not result in 2 separate pieces, but in a new Moebius’ strip, twice the length and half the width. Our first experiment tested whether language of thought, on the premise of it being the basis for all cognitive tasks, is exclusively digital or analogical utilizing mental images. We used 2 groups: a control and an experimental one. In the control group, we presented a Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally partition it. In the experimental group, we presented them with a Moebius’ strip and asked them to mentally represent it, then we removed it from their field of perception, and when the subjects were sure that they could adequately represent it, we asked them to mentally partition it. We hypothesized that the same degree of success between the two groups would support Kosslyn’s views, whereas lower scores in the experimental group would support Pylyshyn’s. What happened was something we did not expect. After reviewing their scores, we found that there was a total failure in either of the two. The subjects’ failure to mentally partition Moebius’ strips led us to a new series of studies, focusing on the particularity of Moebius’ strip and the process of mental partitioning itself. We tried to investigate possible cognitive biases, a pattern of divergent judgements that is activated during mental partitioning and the effect of «Cognitive schemas» during this task. To summarize, we tested whether there are cases where problem solving is guided by established and unchanging «cognitive schemas» that affect not only mental images but also perception (Evans, 2010).
To investigate our hypothesis, we evaluated scores from two different tasks: Hinton’s cube (1979) and Moebius’ strip. Our basic goal was to answer: does established knowledge affect thought and perceptual ability negatively leading to cognitive biases, is Pylyshyn’s theory of cognitive penetrability applicable in the case of mental partitioning of an object (Moebius’ strip) and what are the differences between Pylyshyn’s and Kosslyn’s views on the nature of our mental representations, How do the scores in mental imaging tasks differ from scores in the mental partitioning ones. Our second goal was to investigate the relationship between spatio- visual skill and the ability for mental partitioning in healthy subjects, using the Moebius’ strip, in order to examine the hypothesis that mental partitioning is a sub-skill of mental imaging presenting a high level of difficulty for subjects. Our third goal was to examine whether the influence of personal factors (such as: gender, age, level of education) affects performance in mental partitioning tasks and if scores of a certain group (psychology and architecture students) would be differentiated from the rest of the population.
Research Plan
We carried two studies: The first one attempted to answer our first goal. Initially, we divided our subjects into 2 sub-groups: a) a control group (N=186, 54.1%), in which subjects were presented with a Moebius’strip and were asked to mentally partition the shape they had visual contact with, and b) an experimental group (N=158, 45.9%), in which subjects were presented with a Moebius’ strip while witnessing its creation process. After they confirmed that they could mentally represent it adequately, we removed the strip from their visual field and asked them to mentally partition its mental image. This plan attempted to investigate potential differences between perceptual and mental imaging process in mental manipulation tasks such as the one using the Moebius’ strip. To investigate whether, apart from mental images, people use propositional representations (Pylyshyn’s views),we administered 3 tasks:
In the first task (Hinton 1), the correct answer was based on the subject’s prior knowledge on cubes (propositional representation).
In the second task (Hinton 2), the correct answer was based on the mental manipulation of the cube (analogical representation).
In the third task (Moebius’ strip mental partitioning), we hypothesized that the subjects’ prior knowledge (Whatever you cut gets separated into 2 pieces) would affect them and lead a significant percentage to answer incorrectly, supporting the view that this task involves mainly propositional
and not analogical representations. The second study attempted to confirm our initial hypothesis that the mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip is guided by cognitive penetrability. It attempts to show that mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip, as a mental manipulation task, reaches the limits of human cognition and problem solving. Finally, the second study investigates the effects of three different sub-groups (1.architects, 2.psychologist, 3. Typical population) and of personal factors on spatial and partitioning problem solving.Hypothesis
Study1: The basic hypothesis here is that there are spatial tasks, like mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip that don’t utilize mental images but are subjected to what Pylyshyn (2003) identified as cognitive penetrability. Taking into account that no participant had prior knowledge of Moebius’ strip, the scores in partitioning tasks would not be affected by the perceivable object but by their prior knowledge about how the world works. We predicted that the majority of the subjects would not put enough effort in the task using already established mental schemas and their properties. If the subjects could visualize the two simple shapes (Hilton’s cube and Moebius’ strip), then potential low scores in spatial tasks could hint towards increased difficulty in mental image manipulation. Specifically, regarding the mental partitioning of Moebius strip task answers such as «cutting the strip will give us 2 new strips»could support the idea that mental images depend on an established worldview that is on abstract propositional knowledge and not on perceptual processes (Pylyshyn, 2003). Following the idea mentioned above, these two specific tasks (Hilton’s cube mental manipulation and Moebius’ strip’s mental partitioning) would be based on propositional representations, meaning that mental imaging processes are guided by propositional factors (Hypothesis 1). A second hypothesis states that the mental partitioning task would not be facilitated by the perceptually available Moebius’ strip (Hypothesis 2).
Study 2: Low scores in mental partitioning tasks are expected to confirm the existence of cognitive penetrability (Pylyshyn, 2003) (Hypothesis 1). High correlation is also expected between spatial perception and mental partitioning tasks (Anderson, 1981) (Hypothesis 3). We expect that gender will be a differentiating factor as Voyer, et. al.(1995) found that males score higher in certain spatial tasks, including mental partitioning (Hypothesis 4). Younger subjects are also expected to score higher than older ones (Silverman & Eals, 1992) (Hypothesis 5). Lastly we hypothesized that the group of architects would score higher than the other 2 groups (Voyer, et. al. 1995) (Hypothesis 6).
Study 1 – Participants
In the first part of the study a total of 344 subjects where tested. Of them 157 were male (45.6%) and 187 were female (54.4%). Participants were categorized in 2 different age groups: above 25 years old (born in 1987 or earlier) (N=166,48.3%) and below 25 years old (1988 and after) (N=178, 51.7%), in order to examine age factors in the tasks given.
Study 1 – Tasks
Hinton 1 –Identifying the number of free edges of a mentally depicted Hilton’s cube
We asked the subjects to find the number of free edges in a mental depicted cube (30 cm each side) when that cube was standingon either of its edges
Hilton 2 –Locating the free edges of a mentally depicted Hilton’s cube in space
The second task demanded the mental manipulation of the cube. We asked the subjects to pinpoint the cubes edges in space.
Mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip task
We asked the subjects to imagine what the partitioning of a Moebius strip along its axon would give us. They were also given information about the strip’s creation process
Study 2 – Participants
A total of 154 subjects participated in this study. They were divided in 3 sub-groups : a ) a group of psychology students, b) a group of architecture students, c)and a typical population group. Specifically, 48 participants were architecture students (4th or 5th year) from the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 33 were psychology students (3rd year or above) from the Aristotle University of Thessaloniki and the rest came from typical population. Regarding gender, 77 were male and 77 female. Regarding age, 2 groups were formed : a) above 25 years old (N=77, M=53.8) and below 25 years old (N=77, M=21.2). 2 groups were also formed based on educational level : a)12 years or below (N=41), b) 13 years or above(N=113). Our sample did not include people with language or other problems (visual, auditory, psychopathology, etc.)
Study 2 – Tasks
Spatial Ability Tasks
Ropes: We presented our participants with a picture of 6 different ropes entangled together. We asked them to tell us if each rope would create a knot when pulled.Each success was awarded with 1 point
Mental Paper Folding: We presented 20 instances where it was required from the participants to mentally fold a sheet of paper, pierce it and choose the correct solution from 5 alternatives as to how the holes would be arrayed if we unfolded that particular (Ekstrom,et. al., 1976). Each success was awarded with 1 point
Hilton’s Cube – 2: The task is the same as the one described in Study 1. We awarded 14 points if all the edges were correctly arrayed in space (14 point = max score), 7 points if the participant arrayed at least 3 edges correctly and 0 points if the participant arrayed less than 3 correctly.
The total amount of points a participant could gather in the second study was 40.
Mental Partitioning Tasks
The tasks administered were created specifically for the goals of the present study based on previous findings (Kargopoulos, 2009). Cranach’s a was deemed sufficient(a=.81).
Mental partitioning of the course of 2 trains along a Moebius’ strip: We presented our subjects with a Moebius’ strip (3cm width, 25cm length).We asked them to imagine the reverse lengthwise course of two train starting from six (6) different points of origin on the strip. The participants had to answer if the 2 trains would eventually meet on the strip. One point was awarded for each success
Mental partitioning of the angle of an analogical clock’s indicators: We asked the participants to mentally partition the angle created by the
indicators on an analogical clock. The task had 20 items and one point was awarded for each success.
Mental partitioning of a Moebius’ strip: We presented our participants with the process of creating a Moebius’ strip and asked them to describe the shape produced if we cut the strip lengthwise. One point was awarded for each success.
Mental partitioning of a Moebius’ strip with alternatives: Each participant was presented with the strip and ask to mentally partition it lengthwise starting from a random point of origin. When they were finished they had to choose from 9 different alternative choices regarding the new shape produced. The answers were coded as «correct» or «incorrect» for the purpose of Binomial Test.
Statistical Analysis
We used the SPSS software package in order to analyze our data. To study the relationship between our variables we used the following techniques: one-way anova, binomial test, t-test to examine quantitive variables, chi squared independence test to examine categorical variables and adjusted standardized residuals to examine statistically significant chi-square results. For large samples, adjusted standardized residuals approach normal distribution. Positive adjusted standardized residuals >1.96 indicate statistically significant relationship between the variables in those cells.
From the 344 participants, 160 (46.5%) answered correctly in the Hilton 1 task , 31
(9%) answered correctly in the Hilton 2 task and 9 participant (2.6%) correctly partitioned Moebius’ strip (Table 1). Gender and age did not affect scores in the visual – spatial tasks (Hinton 1 and Hinton 2). Gender and age effects are present in the Moebius’ strip partitioning task. The correct answer was given by 6 males (1.7%) and 3 females (0.87%). Younger participants (<25>25 group. From a total of 9 correct answers, 6 (1.7%) came from the younger group and 3 (0.87%)from the older. There were 7 correct answers in the control group (the group that were in visual contact with the Moebius’ strip) and 2 correct answers in the experimental one (Table 2).
From these results we can infer that success rates in the mental partitioning tasks were low. A significant rate of success was present in the younger group. The group that had visual contact with the Moebius’ strip had a higher rate of success than the group that could only mentally manipulate without seeing it, however this result cannot support the idea that mental manipulation is facilitated by the perceptual condition Performance relationship to Hinton 1 and Hinton 2 tasks To identify the statistical significance of the relationship between performance of the participants in Hinton 1 (Propositional representation) and Hinton 2 (Analogical representation) tasks, a chi-squared independence check was applied. There was a statistically significant correlation between performance in Hinton 1 and Hinton 2 tasks, chi-squared (N = 129, BE = 1) = 39.18, p <.001. Of the 160 participants who responded correctly to the Hinton 1 task , only 31 (24.03%) responded correctly to Hinton 2 (Analogical representation)(see Table 3).It seems that most people who responded correctly to the first task had mistakenly manipulated the mental image in the second one (Hinton 2). This confirms Hypothesis 1.
Performance relationship between mental partitioning of Moebius strip and Hinton 1 tasks
There was a statistically significant relationship in participants’ performance between mental partitioning and Hinton 1 tasks, chi-squared (Ν = 9, ΒΕ=1) = 10.62, p< .001. Those who responded correctly to mental partition tasks were also able to find the
correct number of vertices in Hinton 1 (Propositional representation) task (N=9) (see Table 4).
Performance relationship between mental partitioning of Moebius strip and Hinton 2 tasks
There was a statistically significant relationship of participant performance between the Moebius strip and Hinton 2 tasks, chi-squared (Ν = 24, ΒΕ = 1) = 53.29, p < .001. Of the 31 subjects who responded correctly to the Hinton 2 (Analogical representation) task, 24 (77, 5%) gave the wrong answer to the mental partitioning task. This confirms Hypothesis 1 (see Table 5).
In order to examine the hypothesis that perceptual processing does not facilitate the mental processes in partitioning of the Moebius strip task (Hypothesis 2), we compared the performance of the two experimental groups.For the statistically significant relationship in the performance between the two groups to be identified, the chi-squared independence check was applied. There was a statisticaly significant correlation in performance between the experimental and control groups, chi-sqaured (N=156, BE=1) = 43.59, p<.001. Of the 186 subjects who had visual contact with the strip while trying to partition it mentally (control group), 179 (96.24%) gave the wrong answer and of the 158 subjects who tried partitioning its mental image (experimental group),156 (98.74%) gave also the wrong answer (see Table 6). Binomial test for success in mental partitioning task showed that there’s a statistically significant difference in the percentage of those who failed in the control (p<.001) and the experimental groups (p<.001). The two groups showed no differences in their performance. This confirms Hypothesis 2.
Relationship between age and performance in Hinton 1, Hinton 2 and mental partition of Moebius strip tasks.
According to the results from the first two tasks (Hinton1 and 2) there were no significant differences in performance amongst subjects from the two age groups. Of the 9 subjects who responded correctly to the Moebius strip task, the younger (under 25) showed better performance (6 subjects, 66.7%).The Binomial test for success or failure in each age group in Moebius strip task showed a statistically significant difference for younger subjects (p < .001).
Relationship between gender and performance in Hinton 1, Hinton 2 and Moebiusstrip tasks.According to the results from the first two tasks (Hinton 1 and 2) there were no significant differences in performance between men and women. Of the 9 subjects, however, who responded correctly in the mental partitioning task, men, showed a better performance (6 subjects, 66.7%). The binomial test for successor failure between men and women in the Moebius strip task showed that there was a statistically significant difference over men (p < .001). Summary of the results from the first study According to the results from the first study, only 46, 5% of the participants responded correctly in the Hinton 1 task. Assuming that when they tried to solve Hinton 1 task, people used a virtual mental representation, then we could say that it didn’t help them to answer correctly. We claim that the unusual position of the cube and its vertices makes its virtual representation a quite difficult task, we also claim that those who gave the correct answer either used the propositional representation: «The cube has eight vertices, if I remove two then six remain» or they were really good at manipulating the cube’s mental image. If the latter were true, then those participants should have showed a higher rate of success in the task that followed, which clearly required the mental manipulation of the inverted cube’s image (Hinton 2). It seems that there are spatial tasks that in order to be solved need not only the manipulation of a virtual representation but most importantly the use of some prior knowledge in the form of propositional representation. Subjects in their vast majority found it more difficult to manipulate the cube’s mental image in the second task. Of the 344 participants, only 31 managed to give the correct number of vertices in space. Though people had a hard time manipulating the cube’s mental image, their success rates were much higher for the Hinton 1 task in which propositional representation was more accessible. Only 9 of the 344 participants could find the correct answer for the Moebius strip task in which mental manipulation of the strip image was impossible. This task seems to have been catalyzed by the prior knowledge: «Whatever you cut gets separated into two pieces» (Cognitive penetrability) which led 97,3% of the participants to the wrong answer. With regard to the variables we examined in mental partitioning it seems that age and gender tend to influence performance with young (under 25) men being better, though the small number of participants who responded correctly prevents us from further supporting this case. One particular interest was the fact that the condition of having visual contact with the strip while trying to mentally partition it did not increase the percentage of correct responses significantly. This means that Pylyshyn’s cognitive penetrability should be also applied to perceptual phenomena (Kargopoulos, 2009). The experimental groups comparison shows that participants who were in visual contact with the Moebius strip showed a better performance, though these results cannot support that mental partition is facilitated by being in perceptual contact with the object.
Study 2
The relationship in overall performance between visual competence and mental partitioning tasks.
In order to investigate the correlation in overall performance between visual competence and mental partitioning tasks we applied Pearson correlation coefficient. The overall performance in visual competence tasks showed a statistically significant correlation with performance in mental partitioning tasks r = .75, p <.001. There was a positive correlation to performance in visual competence and mental partitioning tasks. This confirms Hypothesis 3.
The relationship between profession and overall performance in visual competence tasks.
Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in 3 visual competence tasks between the three different profession groups showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of the profession factor was statistically significant [F(2.151) = 12.64, p = . 001]. After making adjustments according to Bonferroni, regarding the number of comparisons we found statistically significant differences between the mean scores of the professions, paired t = (104) = 1.37, two-way p< .001. The mean scores of the architecture group (Μ = 24.02, SD = 7.2) were significantly higher than those of the typical population (Μ = 18.88, SD = 7.03) and psychology groups alike (Μ = 16.97, SD = 5.57). The architecture students had higher scores than the psychology students and the people working typical jobs. This partially confirms Hypothesis 6.
Relationship between profession and overall performance in mental partitioning tasks.
Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in mental partitioning tasks between the three different profession groups showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of the profession factor was statistically significant [F (2.151) = 3.04,p < . 005]. After making adjustments according to Bonferroni, about the number of the number of comparisons, we found statistically significant differences between the mean scores of the professionst(104) = 1.54, δίπλευρη p < .005). The mean scores of the architecture group (ΜΟ = 15.19, ΤΑ = 8.27) were significantly higher than those of the typical population (ΜΟ=12.37, ΤΑ=6.94 and psychology group (ΜΟ=11.58, ΤΑ=6.53).This confirms Hypothesis7.
Relationship between age and overall performance in spatial reasoning tasks
Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in spatial reasoning tasks between the 2 age groups (above 25 and below 25) showed statistically significant results. Specifically, the effect of age factor was statistically significant (F151= 14.34, p<. 000). The mean scores for «below 25» age group (ΜΟ = 22.05, ΤΑ = 7.93) were statistically higher than those of the «above 25» group (ΜΟ = 17.76, ΤΑ = 5.73). Participants that were below 25 years old scored higher in spatial reasoning tasks.Relationship between age and overall performance in mental partitioning tasks
Anova analysis to compare the mean scores in mental partitioning tasks between the 2 age groups (above 25 and below 25) showed statistically significant results.
Specifically, the effect of age factor was statistically significant (F152 = 6.96, p<.000). The mean scores for «below 25 age» group (ΜΟ = 14.51, ΤΑ = 8.48) were statistically higher than that of the «above 25» group (ΜΟ = 11.41, ΤΑ = 5.49). Participants that were below 25 years old scored higher in mental partitioning tasks. This confirms Hypothesis 6.
Task: «Alternative solutions to mental partitioning of Moebius’ strip»
For the purposes of this study, the answers provided in the task were participants had to choose from 9 alternative solutions to the mental partitioning of Moebius strip, were of particular interest
Out of the 154 participants, 137 (89%) chose a wrong answer revolving around the separation of the strip into 2 distinct pieces (answers: 1, 3, 5, 7, 9). 17 participants (11%) chose answers that had only 1 piece as the product of mental partitioning (answers: 2, 4, 6, 8), and of the 17, only 8 (5,2% from a total of Ν=154) gave the correct answer (second option). Biniomial tests for the success or failure in the task of ’Alternative solutions to Moebius’ strip mental partitioning’ showed statistically significant differences over the participant’s failure rate(p < .001). The percentage of participants who were close to the correct answer was statistically smaller than the percentage of those who provided a wrong answer. This result confirm sour hypothesis about the existence of strong cognitive biases and of cognitive penetrability in the specific task, affirming our initial hypothesis (Hypothesis 1). At this point we must stress out that the most chosen option among the alternatives provided was the propositional representation: «whatever you cut gets separated into 2 new pieces».The alternative 5 option, according to which cutting the strip lengthwise will give 2 new strips of equal length but half the width of the original, gathered the highest percentage of answers (67.5%). The alternative answer 8 concerning one whole piece that participants couldn’t identify gathered 0%. This result supports the idea that participants used the propositional knowledge and didn’t put any effort in mentally manipulating the picture. We can also observe that among the 154 participants in the 2nd study, 137 (89%) chose between answers concerning 2 pieces and 17 participants (11%) chose between alternatives regarding 1 piece as a product of mental partitioning. This leads us to believe that prior established knowledge and not mental manipulation of images guided the majority of our sample in false answers regarding the mental partitioning task, making obvious the existence of cognitive biases guiding thought during this task’s problem solving process. This also confirm sour 1st Hypothesis.
Summary of results from the second study
From the results of the 2nd study, we conclude that there is a significant relationship between performance in spatial and mental partitioning tasks. Architecture students and participants below 25 years of age scored higher in both tasks. It seems that age and profession affects spatial reasoning competence. Τhe task «Alternative solutions to Moebius’ strip mental partitioning» hints towards the use of propositional thought as well as the guidance of this task’s representation by prior knowledge about the world. Specifically, whereas in the first task, success rate was 2.6%, when alternative answers were provided it doubled (5,2 %).However, the success rate was still considerably low. A wide percentage of the answers provided (89%), revolved around the result of 2 separate pieces, leading us to believe that previous knowledge as to the nature of partitioning affected the participants’ answers. These results provide evidence that the ability to mentally partition an object is cognitively penetrable, but also hints towards a strong cognitive bias that affected a high percentage of our sample. The most common option chosen amongst alternatives hints towards our initial hypothesis: our inner propositional representation that cutting an object leads to 2 new seperate pieces will guide us towards the option: «2 new strips our produced» which gathered 67.5% of the total answers. These results provide evidence regarding that most of the participants used their propositional knowledge while putting little to no effort in mentally manipulating the strip.
The low rates of correct answers in the 2 studies (a total of 498 subjects, with only 2.6% of them finding the correct answer), for both participants who could see the strip as well as those who tried to mentally manipulate it, leads us to the conclusion that cognitive penetrability, as defined by Pylyshyn (2003), guides mental transformation. We can also expand the idea of cognitive penetrability to perception. The large failure rate in the mental partitioning tasks supports the idea that some mental transformation problems are not facilitated by prior knowledge. The participants didn’t seem to manipulate the strip’s mental image, and even if we accept that they manipulated it, the majority failed at the task. The basic principle of propositional representation isthat words and images are represented in an abstract way that suggests the meaning and the use of knowledge (Pylyshyn, 2003). People encode and use all information (verbal and nonverbal) in the form of propositional representations. When we want to use this information, we recall the corresponding propositional representation and use it for the appropriate tasks. The information: «Whatever you cut gets separated into 2 pieces» is a propositional unit that consists of two modules: 1st module = «Whatever you cut» and 2nd module = «two pieces» are connected by the relationship of «gets separated into». This rule seems to have affected the participants and validated Pylyshyn’s views that mental images are guided by propositional cognitive cues (cognitive penetrability), at least regarding the task of the mental partitioning of Moebius strip. The participants had also difficulties in mentally manipulating the perceivable strip. They mustered available cognitive schemas (a simple collar) and tried to create semantic relationships between the old and new knowledge in order tounderstand and find the solution to the task at hand. Whatever the participants tried to manipulate, concerning the Moebius strip task was affected by prior knowledge (the schema: «Whatever you cut, gets separated into 2 pieces»), which confirms Neisser’s theory (1997). Shepard & Metzler’s (1971) views, that people execute various mental tree – dimensional transformations cannot be supported by this study’s findings. The low success rates in the mental partitioning tasks lead us to the conclusion that the law of the least possible mental effort is true. People will use a spontaneous / fast way of thinking (Kahneman, 2011). The participants used a fast and spontaneous system of thinking, which works- based on associations, with little to no effort, without the use of self-regulation, based on cognitive biases, with limited use of typical logic, and for that reason people came to false conclusions. Accordingto our results, the independent variables «age», «profession» and «education level» have an effect on spatial perception and mental partitioning tasks. The architecture students group and the «below 25» group scored the highest, and the psychology student group scored the lowest. The factor «education level» affects only the spatial perception tasks and not the mental partitioning ones, with higher education level participants scoring higher. Scores in spatial perception tasks are a predictor for performance in mental partitioning tasks and vice versa. The participants displayed a wide variety of cognitive behaviors during the tasks administered that hinted towards limitations of human thought. Their answers were a product of prior knowledge (biases), which prevented them from using possibly more effective solution methods. Scores in the main mental partitioning task showed that spatial reasoning is affected by what people know, and not by what they see or manipulate. These results lead us to support the idea that the process of mental partitioning a novel, real object rests on propositional representations and not on visual images. The participants’ thought was anchored in the idea that cutting an object leads to 2 seperate pieces and prevented them from approaching the problem from a different perspective. In this specific partitioning sub-task, subjects sought a fast approach, that would connect their goal with available to them information. They used prior knowledge, on the basis that this would facilitate problem solving. People manipulating a Moebius’ strip seemed to see what they know, and not what ‘’there is’’. These findings support Pylyshyn’s ideas, who supported the presence of specific conceptual – shaping processing. Ιn particular the position that virtual processe sare guided by cognitive propositional elements (Pylyshyn, 2003). It seems that a series of «up down» information processes affect our perception, whereas our experiences create our internal theories and the way we perceive the world, at least in the case of visual – constructive transformation tasks(ex. Moebius’ strip’s mental partitioning).The study of cognitive penetrability and the mental partition of mental and real object such as Moebius’ strip constitute fertile ground for further inquiries in Cognitive science. Our findings bring back to the scientific background the idea that the mind’s selective attentionto previous experience and cognitive schemas will decidedly affect human thought.
Clearly Auctoresonline and particularly Psychology and Mental Health Care Journal is dedicated to improving health care services for individuals and populations. The editorial boards' ability to efficiently recognize and share the global importance of health literacy with a variety of stakeholders. Auctoresonline publishing platform can be used to facilitate of optimal client-based services and should be added to health care professionals' repertoire of evidence-based health care resources.
Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Intervention The submission and review process was adequate. However I think that the publication total value should have been enlightened in early fases. Thank you for all.
Journal of Women Health Care and Issues By the present mail, I want to say thank to you and tour colleagues for facilitating my published article. Specially thank you for the peer review process, support from the editorial office. I appreciate positively the quality of your journal.
Journal of Clinical Research and Reports I would be very delighted to submit my testimonial regarding the reviewer board and the editorial office. The reviewer board were accurate and helpful regarding any modifications for my manuscript. And the editorial office were very helpful and supportive in contacting and monitoring with any update and offering help. It was my pleasure to contribute with your promising Journal and I am looking forward for more collaboration.
We would like to thank the Journal of Thoracic Disease and Cardiothoracic Surgery because of the services they provided us for our articles. The peer-review process was done in a very excellent time manner, and the opinions of the reviewers helped us to improve our manuscript further. The editorial office had an outstanding correspondence with us and guided us in many ways. During a hard time of the pandemic that is affecting every one of us tremendously, the editorial office helped us make everything easier for publishing scientific work. Hope for a more scientific relationship with your Journal.
The peer-review process which consisted high quality queries on the paper. I did answer six reviewers’ questions and comments before the paper was accepted. The support from the editorial office is excellent.
Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery. I had the experience of publishing a research article recently. The whole process was simple from submission to publication. The reviewers made specific and valuable recommendations and corrections that improved the quality of my publication. I strongly recommend this Journal.
Dr. Katarzyna Byczkowska My testimonial covering: "The peer review process is quick and effective. The support from the editorial office is very professional and friendly. Quality of the Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions is scientific and publishes ground-breaking research on cardiology that is useful for other professionals in the field.
Thank you most sincerely, with regard to the support you have given in relation to the reviewing process and the processing of my article entitled "Large Cell Neuroendocrine Carcinoma of The Prostate Gland: A Review and Update" for publication in your esteemed Journal, Journal of Cancer Research and Cellular Therapeutics". The editorial team has been very supportive.
Testimony of Journal of Clinical Otorhinolaryngology: work with your Reviews has been a educational and constructive experience. The editorial office were very helpful and supportive. It was a pleasure to contribute to your Journal.
Dr. Bernard Terkimbi Utoo, I am happy to publish my scientific work in Journal of Women Health Care and Issues (JWHCI). The manuscript submission was seamless and peer review process was top notch. I was amazed that 4 reviewers worked on the manuscript which made it a highly technical, standard and excellent quality paper. I appreciate the format and consideration for the APC as well as the speed of publication. It is my pleasure to continue with this scientific relationship with the esteem JWHCI.
This is an acknowledgment for peer reviewers, editorial board of Journal of Clinical Research and Reports. They show a lot of consideration for us as publishers for our research article “Evaluation of the different factors associated with side effects of COVID-19 vaccination on medical students, Mutah university, Al-Karak, Jordan”, in a very professional and easy way. This journal is one of outstanding medical journal.
Dear Hao Jiang, to Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing We greatly appreciate the efficient, professional and rapid processing of our paper by your team. If there is anything else we should do, please do not hesitate to let us know. On behalf of my co-authors, we would like to express our great appreciation to editor and reviewers.
As an author who has recently published in the journal "Brain and Neurological Disorders". I am delighted to provide a testimonial on the peer review process, editorial office support, and the overall quality of the journal. The peer review process at Brain and Neurological Disorders is rigorous and meticulous, ensuring that only high-quality, evidence-based research is published. The reviewers are experts in their fields, and their comments and suggestions were constructive and helped improve the quality of my manuscript. The review process was timely and efficient, with clear communication from the editorial office at each stage. The support from the editorial office was exceptional throughout the entire process. The editorial staff was responsive, professional, and always willing to help. They provided valuable guidance on formatting, structure, and ethical considerations, making the submission process seamless. Moreover, they kept me informed about the status of my manuscript and provided timely updates, which made the process less stressful. The journal Brain and Neurological Disorders is of the highest quality, with a strong focus on publishing cutting-edge research in the field of neurology. The articles published in this journal are well-researched, rigorously peer-reviewed, and written by experts in the field. The journal maintains high standards, ensuring that readers are provided with the most up-to-date and reliable information on brain and neurological disorders. In conclusion, I had a wonderful experience publishing in Brain and Neurological Disorders. The peer review process was thorough, the editorial office provided exceptional support, and the journal's quality is second to none. I would highly recommend this journal to any researcher working in the field of neurology and brain disorders.
Dear Agrippa Hilda, Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery, Editorial Coordinator, I trust this message finds you well. I want to extend my appreciation for considering my article for publication in your esteemed journal. I am pleased to provide a testimonial regarding the peer review process and the support received from your editorial office. The peer review process for my paper was carried out in a highly professional and thorough manner. The feedback and comments provided by the authors were constructive and very useful in improving the quality of the manuscript. This rigorous assessment process undoubtedly contributes to the high standards maintained by your journal.
International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews. I strongly recommend to consider submitting your work to this high-quality journal. The support and availability of the Editorial staff is outstanding and the review process was both efficient and rigorous.
Thank you very much for publishing my Research Article titled “Comparing Treatment Outcome Of Allergic Rhinitis Patients After Using Fluticasone Nasal Spray And Nasal Douching" in the Journal of Clinical Otorhinolaryngology. As Medical Professionals we are immensely benefited from study of various informative Articles and Papers published in this high quality Journal. I look forward to enriching my knowledge by regular study of the Journal and contribute my future work in the field of ENT through the Journal for use by the medical fraternity. The support from the Editorial office was excellent and very prompt. I also welcome the comments received from the readers of my Research Article.
Dear Erica Kelsey, Editorial Coordinator of Cancer Research and Cellular Therapeutics Our team is very satisfied with the processing of our paper by your journal. That was fast, efficient, rigorous, but without unnecessary complications. We appreciated the very short time between the submission of the paper and its publication on line on your site.
I am very glad to say that the peer review process is very successful and fast and support from the Editorial Office. Therefore, I would like to continue our scientific relationship for a long time. And I especially thank you for your kindly attention towards my article. Have a good day!
"We recently published an article entitled “Influence of beta-Cyclodextrins upon the Degradation of Carbofuran Derivatives under Alkaline Conditions" in the Journal of “Pesticides and Biofertilizers” to show that the cyclodextrins protect the carbamates increasing their half-life time in the presence of basic conditions This will be very helpful to understand carbofuran behaviour in the analytical, agro-environmental and food areas. We greatly appreciated the interaction with the editor and the editorial team; we were particularly well accompanied during the course of the revision process, since all various steps towards publication were short and without delay".
I would like to express my gratitude towards you process of article review and submission. I found this to be very fair and expedient. Your follow up has been excellent. I have many publications in national and international journal and your process has been one of the best so far. Keep up the great work.
We are grateful for this opportunity to provide a glowing recommendation to the Journal of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy. We found that the editorial team were very supportive, helpful, kept us abreast of timelines and over all very professional in nature. The peer review process was rigorous, efficient and constructive that really enhanced our article submission. The experience with this journal remains one of our best ever and we look forward to providing future submissions in the near future.
I am very pleased to serve as EBM of the journal, I hope many years of my experience in stem cells can help the journal from one way or another. As we know, stem cells hold great potential for regenerative medicine, which are mostly used to promote the repair response of diseased, dysfunctional or injured tissue using stem cells or their derivatives. I think Stem Cell Research and Therapeutics International is a great platform to publish and share the understanding towards the biology and translational or clinical application of stem cells.
I would like to give my testimony in the support I have got by the peer review process and to support the editorial office where they were of asset to support young author like me to be encouraged to publish their work in your respected journal and globalize and share knowledge across the globe. I really give my great gratitude to your journal and the peer review including the editorial office.
I am delighted to publish our manuscript entitled "A Perspective on Cocaine Induced Stroke - Its Mechanisms and Management" in the Journal of Neuroscience and Neurological Surgery. The peer review process, support from the editorial office, and quality of the journal are excellent. The manuscripts published are of high quality and of excellent scientific value. I recommend this journal very much to colleagues.
Dr.Tania Muñoz, My experience as researcher and author of a review article in The Journal Clinical Cardiology and Interventions has been very enriching and stimulating. The editorial team is excellent, performs its work with absolute responsibility and delivery. They are proactive, dynamic and receptive to all proposals. Supporting at all times the vast universe of authors who choose them as an option for publication. The team of review specialists, members of the editorial board, are brilliant professionals, with remarkable performance in medical research and scientific methodology. Together they form a frontline team that consolidates the JCCI as a magnificent option for the publication and review of high-level medical articles and broad collective interest. I am honored to be able to share my review article and open to receive all your comments.
“The peer review process of JPMHC is quick and effective. Authors are benefited by good and professional reviewers with huge experience in the field of psychology and mental health. The support from the editorial office is very professional. People to contact to are friendly and happy to help and assist any query authors might have. Quality of the Journal is scientific and publishes ground-breaking research on mental health that is useful for other professionals in the field”.
Dear editorial department: On behalf of our team, I hereby certify the reliability and superiority of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews in the peer review process, editorial support, and journal quality. Firstly, the peer review process of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is rigorous, fair, transparent, fast, and of high quality. The editorial department invites experts from relevant fields as anonymous reviewers to review all submitted manuscripts. These experts have rich academic backgrounds and experience, and can accurately evaluate the academic quality, originality, and suitability of manuscripts. The editorial department is committed to ensuring the rigor of the peer review process, while also making every effort to ensure a fast review cycle to meet the needs of authors and the academic community. Secondly, the editorial team of the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is composed of a group of senior scholars and professionals with rich experience and professional knowledge in related fields. The editorial department is committed to assisting authors in improving their manuscripts, ensuring their academic accuracy, clarity, and completeness. Editors actively collaborate with authors, providing useful suggestions and feedback to promote the improvement and development of the manuscript. We believe that the support of the editorial department is one of the key factors in ensuring the quality of the journal. Finally, the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is renowned for its high- quality articles and strict academic standards. The editorial department is committed to publishing innovative and academically valuable research results to promote the development and progress of related fields. The International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews is reasonably priced and ensures excellent service and quality ratio, allowing authors to obtain high-level academic publishing opportunities in an affordable manner. I hereby solemnly declare that the International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews has a high level of credibility and superiority in terms of peer review process, editorial support, reasonable fees, and journal quality. Sincerely, Rui Tao.
Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions I testity the covering of the peer review process, support from the editorial office, and quality of the journal.
Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, we deeply appreciate the interest shown in our work and its publication. It has been a true pleasure to collaborate with you. The peer review process, as well as the support provided by the editorial office, have been exceptional, and the quality of the journal is very high, which was a determining factor in our decision to publish with you.
The peer reviewers process is quick and effective, the supports from editorial office is excellent, the quality of journal is high. I would like to collabroate with Internatioanl journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews journal clinically in the future time.
Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, I would like to express my sincerest gratitude for the trust placed in our team for the publication in your journal. It has been a true pleasure to collaborate with you on this project. I am pleased to inform you that both the peer review process and the attention from the editorial coordination have been excellent. Your team has worked with dedication and professionalism to ensure that your publication meets the highest standards of quality. We are confident that this collaboration will result in mutual success, and we are eager to see the fruits of this shared effort.
Dear Dr. Jessica Magne, Editorial Coordinator 0f Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, I hope this message finds you well. I want to express my utmost gratitude for your excellent work and for the dedication and speed in the publication process of my article titled "Navigating Innovation: Qualitative Insights on Using Technology for Health Education in Acute Coronary Syndrome Patients." I am very satisfied with the peer review process, the support from the editorial office, and the quality of the journal. I hope we can maintain our scientific relationship in the long term.
Dear Monica Gissare, - Editorial Coordinator of Nutrition and Food Processing. ¨My testimony with you is truly professional, with a positive response regarding the follow-up of the article and its review, you took into account my qualities and the importance of the topic¨.
Dear Dr. Jessica Magne, Editorial Coordinator 0f Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions, The review process for the article “The Handling of Anti-aggregants and Anticoagulants in the Oncologic Heart Patient Submitted to Surgery” was extremely rigorous and detailed. From the initial submission to the final acceptance, the editorial team at the “Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions” demonstrated a high level of professionalism and dedication. The reviewers provided constructive and detailed feedback, which was essential for improving the quality of our work. Communication was always clear and efficient, ensuring that all our questions were promptly addressed. The quality of the “Journal of Clinical Cardiology and Cardiovascular Interventions” is undeniable. It is a peer-reviewed, open-access publication dedicated exclusively to disseminating high-quality research in the field of clinical cardiology and cardiovascular interventions. The journal's impact factor is currently under evaluation, and it is indexed in reputable databases, which further reinforces its credibility and relevance in the scientific field. I highly recommend this journal to researchers looking for a reputable platform to publish their studies.
Dear Editorial Coordinator of the Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing! "I would like to thank the Journal of Nutrition and Food Processing for including and publishing my article. The peer review process was very quick, movement and precise. The Editorial Board has done an extremely conscientious job with much help, valuable comments and advices. I find the journal very valuable from a professional point of view, thank you very much for allowing me to be part of it and I would like to participate in the future!”
Dealing with The Journal of Neurology and Neurological Surgery was very smooth and comprehensive. The office staff took time to address my needs and the response from editors and the office was prompt and fair. I certainly hope to publish with this journal again.Their professionalism is apparent and more than satisfactory. Susan Weiner
My Testimonial Covering as fellowing: Lin-Show Chin. The peer reviewers process is quick and effective, the supports from editorial office is excellent, the quality of journal is high. I would like to collabroate with Internatioanl journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews.
My experience publishing in Psychology and Mental Health Care was exceptional. The peer review process was rigorous and constructive, with reviewers providing valuable insights that helped enhance the quality of our work. The editorial team was highly supportive and responsive, making the submission process smooth and efficient. The journal's commitment to high standards and academic rigor makes it a respected platform for quality research. I am grateful for the opportunity to publish in such a reputable journal.
My experience publishing in International Journal of Clinical Case Reports and Reviews was exceptional. I Come forth to Provide a Testimonial Covering the Peer Review Process and the editorial office for the Professional and Impartial Evaluation of the Manuscript.